martes, 6 de noviembre de 2012

Study shows capital at home matters more than capital at school

Study shows capital at home matters more than capital at school

study published in Research in Social Stratification and Mobility finds that family social capital, described as the bonds between parents and children, such as trust, open lines of communication, and active engagement in a child's academic life, is a more significant factor than the qualities of the school itself with regard to a child's academic achievement.

Using data from the National Longitudinal Education study and structural equation modeling, researchers examined whether the social capital of the home and school social capital (such as extracurricular activities and the ability of teachers to address the needs of individual students) have differing effects on children's academic achievement. Results show that capital from each context promotes achievement, but that students with high levels of family social capital but low levels of school social capital performed better in school than students with high levels of school social capital and low family social capital. Family social capital continues to have a stronger influence on children's academic achievement, even after controlling for socioeconomic status and other demographic characteristics.  
 
 
Can cash incentives lead to positive outcomes for teens?
Using a randomized control trial research design, MDRC is conducting an evaluation of the Opportunity NYC-Family Rewards program. Implemented in 2007, this program offered monetary incentives to families living in poverty for education, health, and workforce participation and job-training activities, with the ultimate goal of breaking the cycle of poverty. In MDRC's most recent report, researchers examine how parents and their teenage children were affected by Family Rewards two years into the program. Their analyses focus on the differences between a treatment group and control group in areas such as time use, mental health, and risky behaviors, as measured by surveys.

Findings of their study show that Family Rewards:
  • Changed how teenagers spent their time. For a subgroup of academically proficient teenagers, it increased the proportion of those who engaged primarily in academic activities and reduced the proportion who engaged primarily in social activities;
  • Increased parents' spending on school-related and leisure expenses and increased the proportion of parents who saved for their children's future education;
  • Had no effects on parents' monitoring of their teenage children's activities or behavior and did not increase parent-teenager conflict or teenagers' depression or anxiety;
  • Had no effects on teenagers' sense of academic competence or their engagement in school, but substantially reduced their self-reported problem behavior, such as aggression and substance use;
  • Did not reduce teenagers' intrinsic motivation by paying them rewards for school attendance and academic achievement.
MDRC's next report on Family Rewards will examine the results after three years of the program; a final report will include two years of post-program follow-up.
 
Text messaging does not affect children's grammatical development
Researchers from Coventry University in England carried out a longitudinal study to investigate whether "text speak" had any detrimental impact on grammatical development and other related literacy and language skills over the course of a year. They assessed the spelling, grammar, understanding of English, and IQ of three groups of children and young people (83 primary school children, 78 secondary school children, and 49 undergraduates), and compared those skills with a sample of their text messages. There was no evidence of any significant relationships between poor grammar in text messages and their understanding of written or spoken grammar. For the primary school children, there was an association between punctuation errors in text messages and spelling ability. Children who made fewer punctuation errors when texting tended to be better at spelling and quicker to process writing than those who made more errors in their text messages. For the undergraduate group, there was some evidence of a link between punctuation errors in text messages and the spelling ability and grammatical understanding of participants. However, this link was weak, and researchers concluded that it was probably related to children's IQ score.
 
Does school entrance age matter?
Researchers in Croatia explored the relationship between the age that students begin school and school achievement. They found only a weak relationship in the lower grades of primary school, and at the end of primary schooling the effects are no longer evident. The study looked at the achievement of fourth- and eighth-grade students in 844 primary schools in Croatia. Students were divided into groups of younger and older school entrants based on the difference between their year of birth and the year of school entry. In the fourth grade, older entrants performed slightly better in all subjects than those who were younger when they entered school, but these differences in achievement were very small (effect sizes ranged from 0.02 to 0.07). By the eighth grade, there was no difference in achievement between younger and older entrants in the majority of subjects. However, contrary to the fourth grade sample, in the subjects where differences in achievement were found, the younger school entrants outperformed the older school entrants, but the effect sizes were again very small (effect sizes ranged from 0 to 0.12). In both samples, school entrance age explains less than one percent of the variance in school achievement in different subjects.

Effective classroom management practices


Effective classroom management practices

As we mentioned in a special message last week, the fall issue of Better: Evidence-based Education magazine covers the important topic of classroom management. This issue presents a wide range of views and evidence about classroom management from international experts, providing both practical insights and evidence to ultimately enhance our children's learning experience, their contribution to their own learning, and their own long-term outcomes. Articles include:
The full issue is available to subscribers. If you have not already subscribed toBetter, you may do so on the Johns Hopkins University Press website. Subscriptions include electronic access to all archived issues. We have covered a range of topics, from readingmath, and science to effective uses of technologyand health and well-being.
 
 
Study investigates inquiry-based science curricula

This study published in the October issue of Research in Science Educationmeasures the effects of higher level, inquiry-based science curricula on students at primary level in Title I schools. The sample included approximately 3,300 K-3 students from six schools who were assigned to experimental or control conditions (N = 115 total) on a random basis according to class. Students in the experimental condition were exposed to concept-based science curriculum that emphasized "deep learning" though concept mastery and investigation, whereas control classes learned science from traditional school-based curricula. Using standardized measures of achievement, researchers found that all ability groups of students benefited from the science inquiry-based approach to learning that emphasized science concepts, and that there was a positive achievement effect for low socio-economic young children who were exposed to such a curriculum.

Related to this topic, the Johns Hopkins School of Education's Center for Research and Reform in Education completed a review of research in May on effective programs for elementary science. Results of the review support the use of inquiry-oriented programs without science kits, which help teachers to learn and use generic processes such as science-reading integration in their daily science teaching. Use of inquiry-oriented science kits such as FOSS, did not show any benefits for science learning. Limited research on technology approaches such as BrainPop showed positive impacts.
 
Home computer use improves children's academic performance

A paper in Oxford Review of Education examines the link between children's home computer use and their academic performance in reading and math. The study uses data from the nine-year-old cohort of the Growing Up in Ireland survey and a multiple regression model to estimate the effect of home computer use on reading and math test scores, and finds that computer use is associated with increased scores. This result holds after controlling for multiple determinants of school performance, and there is no significant difference in effect for the amount of use.

In addition, the study investigates the effects of different types of computer use and finds that surfing the internet for fun, doing projects for school, and emailing are associated with higher reading and math test scores. Children who are permitted to use the computer unsupervised tend to have higher test scores in math, and instant messaging and downloading music or watching movies are negatively associated with both reading and math test scores. However, while these results indicate significant association with academic performance, the study is not able to establish the direction of causation definitively.
 
Neuromyths in education

Possessing greater general knowledge about the brain does not appear to protect teachers from believing in "neuromyths" - misconceptions about neuroscience research in education. A study reported in Frontiers in Educational Psychologyfound that teachers who are interested in the application of neuroscience findings in the classroom find it difficult to distinguish pseudoscience from scientific facts. They tested 242 primary and secondary school teachers in the UK and the Netherlands with an interest in the neuroscience of learning, using an online survey containing 32 statements about the brain and its influence on learning, of which 15 were neuromyths. Results showed that on average, teachers believed 49 percent of the neuromyths, particularly myths related to commercialized education programs like Brain Gym. One of the most commonly believed myths was "Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinesthetic)," which was believed by over 80 percent of teachers in the study. Although loosely based on scientific fact, these neuromyths may have adverse effects on educational practice, and the study concludes that there is a need for enhanced interdisciplinary communication to reduce such misunderstandings in the future and establish a successful collaboration between neuroscience and education.